- Research
- Open access
- Published:
The coexistence of psychological drivers and deterrents of consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat hamburger patties: evidence from South Africa
Agricultural and Food Economics volume 11, Article number: 52 (2023)
Abstract
The widespread consumption of alternative meat products, such as plant-based alternatives in the hamburger patty market, has provided insights into the potential of commercially produced cultured meat. Evidence from previous alternative meat studies shows mixed results on whether experience with commercially available alternative protein will help to overcome or compound consumers’ concerns about cultured meat. This study investigates the effect that South African consumers’ implicit perceptions developed by experience with and from attitudes about novel products and perceptions about the product could influence their acceptance of cultured meat. South African consumers’ perceptions were estimated using opinion statements, and the level of agreement was ranked on a Likert scale. Composite indices were extracted from these data, which were regressed against consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat burger patties. Results suggest that the implicit perceptions (worldviews) promoted both neophobic and neophilic attitudes, while explicit (product-specific) perceptions indicated that concerns of anticipated social, cultural and economic disruptions may drive non-adoption. The study’s results suggest targeted marketing approaches that can utilise implicit perceptions to promote consumer adoption. Other results indicated the areas of concern that should be addressed to facilitate acceptance and the population groups that could be targeted as early adopters. These results contribute to a better understanding of the potential market for cultured meat in the world's eighth-largest per capita beef consumer and provide insights into drivers and deterrents of cultured meat in an environment that struggles with food security.
Introduction
Globally, consumer acceptance of alternative protein in the burger patty market has increased rapidly in the past decade (Asioli et al. 2022). This trend has been partially driven by consumers’ increasing desire to create a more sustainable food production system, which can contribute to a lower environmental footprint (Quevedo-Silva and Pereira 2022). As conventionally produced burger patties continue to cede market share to plant-based protein alternatives (Ang et al. 2023), questions are beginning to arise on the effect that introducing cultured meatFootnote 1 could have on the livestock industry and its supporting stakeholders. The Italian government has acted on these concerns by developing legislation that would prevent the production of cultured meat production, as it is perceived as a product that threatens the country’s food heritage and agricultural industries (Holland 2023). Conversely, the United States of America’s Food and Drug Administration has approved the commercial production of cultured meat, and efforts to allow commercial production are being pursued (Chasani 2023). Currently, only Singapore and Israel produce cultured meat commercially (Future Meat 2021; The Guardian 2020). The possibility of producing cultured meat patties that are affordable is increasing as food scientists have managed to decrease the cost of production from $325,000 USD (Fountain 2013) to $11.36 USD for a 100 g of in vitro beef (Flaws 2019) and $4.00 USD for a 100 g of in vitro chicken breasts (Terazono 2021). However, market penetration in South Africa is unknown as forecasts on the antecedents for consumer acceptance of cultured meat remain limited.
Competing theories have been adopted as credible guides for forecasting consumers’ future responses to introducing cultured meat to the commercial food sector. Neo-classical economics predicts that food preferences will be driven by rational behaviour that results from a cognitive process guided by utility maximisation (Cembalo et al. 2012). However, given the incomplete information (due to lack of commercial availability) regarding cultured meat’s pricing, taste and potential health risks, this theory’s predictions could be sub-optimal as the rationality of consumers will be limited. Other theories, such as the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen 1991), which predicts consumers’ attitudes, beliefs and emotions as direct and indirect antecedents of both intention and behaviour, have been used. As attitudes are often unstable because an individual can hold both positive and negative attitudes about different aspects of cultured meat (Wilks et al. 2019), the omnivore’s paradox (Fischler 1988) has been applied. This philosophy describes people’s simultaneous aversion and attraction to new food products. It represents the coexisting presence of demand driven by the curiosity for novelty (neophilia) and by caution, at times aversion, concerning the new or the unknown (neophobia) with regard to food (Verneau et al. 2014; Starowich 2020). This study aims to adopt this theory and apply it in predicting the factors that could affect South African consumers’ acceptance of cultured meat burger patties. The study aims to determine the drivers and deterrents of cultured meat burger patty acceptance in South Africa. This is done as consumers tend to hold ambivalent attitudes towards novel food products as expressed in a concurrent desire to adopt novel foods with a higher ethical value, accompanied by a desire to face low or no health, economic, social and quality risks and compromises. It is envisaged that this information will assist cultured meat companies looking to expand market penetration and, ultimately, market share and inform policymakers, the livestock industry and its supporting stakeholders about the potential disruption in the conventional protein market.
A limited but fast-growing number of studies have investigated the effect of the cognitive process on consumers’ willingness to try, consume or purchase cultured meat if made commercially available. The majority of previous studies estimate the effect of consumer psychology using consumers’ higher-order values pertaining to life, such as worldviews or cultures (Lewisch and Riefler 2022; Wilks et al. 2019), personality traits (Piochi et al. 2022; Rombach et al. 2022; Hwang et al. 2020) and perceptions of novel products (Lin-Hi et al. 2022; Quevedo-Silva and Pereira 2022). Other studies (Hamlin et al. 2022; Bryant and Barnett 2020) have focused less on using implicit perceptions by using product-specific (explicit) perceptions calculated using cultured meat neophobia scales. Some studies (Li et al. 2023; Hamlin et al. 2022; Bekker et al. 2021; Gomez Luciano et al. 2019) have investigated the concurrent effect of consumer implicit and explicit perceptions of cultured meat on intended behaviour. Such analytical approaches are valuable because they are more comprehensive as they capture the influence of both aspects (implicit and explicit perceptions) on consumers’ intended behaviour. This is important because the variables measuring consumers’ implicit attitudes capture the heuristics that can guide initial judgement when information is scarce, while the explicit variables take into account the detailed information and are considered to result in contextualised and somewhat rational behavioural responses (De Koning et al. 2020; Gawronski and Bodenhausen 2006). Hence, the current study’s analysis provides information on the possible marketing effort that could prime consumers and the continual marketing effort that should be undertaken to encourage long-term consumers of cultured meat. Examples of studies that have investigated the concurrent effect of implicit and explicit perceptions are Bekker et al. (2017) and Lewisch and Riefler (2022), who confirmed that both implicit and explicit variables have a part to play in influencing intended behaviour with regard to cultured meat; with the former assisting in formulating the latter. As cultured meat enters the commercial food market and credence information becomes available on cultured meat, consumers can make product-specific perceptions that interact with higher-order values in influencing intended behaviour. Li et al. (2023), Hamlin et al. (2022), Bekker et al. (2021) and Gomez Luciano et al. (2019) investigated the concurrent effect of implicit and explicit consumer perceptions of cultured beef. However, these studies have yielded mixed results, which are contrary to theory, as the implicit perceptions were found to have an insignificant effect on planned behaviour. Consequently, this warrants further research on the antecedents of consumer acceptance of cultured meat’s acceptance.
This study uses data collected from South Africa in its analysis. South Africa presents a unique context for consumer research because of the high acceptance of meat analogues in its burger patty market. First, South Africa is the first African country to host two of the world’s largest alternative meat companies (Impossible Foods and Beyond Meat) that dominate the plant-based burger patty landscape (Szejda et al. 2021). Burger patties from these two franchises have found a place in numerous South African fast-food restaurant chains, supermarkets, and other food entities. Second, hamburgers make a good medium of study in South Africa because they are one of the most popular food-away-from-home meals across the country (Stowe et al. 2020). Third, similar to other developing countries, South Africa is likely to continue experiencing consumption and demographic trends (e.g. high urbanisation rates, growth in the demand for meat and a rise in demand for convenience foods) that could promote cultured meat acceptance and ultimate adoption. Lastly, success in the non-commercial production of cultured meat has been reported in South Africa (Stark 2022). Therefore, the information provided in this study may assist industry stakeholders and policy-makers in preparing for a future that may not be too distant (Szejda et al. 2021; Siegrist and Hartmann 2020). Given the market potential, high beef consumption per capita and persistent food insecurity issues driven by climate change, a better understanding of drivers and deterrents of cultured meat is warranted in South Africa.
The rest of the manuscript is organised as follows: Section "Methodology" describes the research methodology, section "Results and discussion" reports and discusses the results, and section "Conclusion" concludes the paper.
Methodology
Data for this study were collected in May 2022. Dynata, a private company maintaining a nationally representative sample of South African residents, recruited the respondents. Participation was anonymous and voluntary, and participants could withdraw without prejudice. University of Arkansas IRB approval (protocol #2202386371) was granted for ethical clearance on March 28, 2022. Each respondent was 18 or older, and consent was acquired before the survey commenced. A total of 956 individuals attempted the survey. The sample was reduced to 658 after responses that did not meet the quality criteria (passing an attention check question and answering all the opinion questions) were removed. This was found to provide adequate representation, given South Africa’s population of 59.39 million people; this sample size is consistent with a marginal error of ± 3.2%.Footnote 2
The survey was administered through and managed via Qualtrics and consisted of three sections. Section one solicited information on the respondents’ socio-economic attributes. This included their race, education, gender, income, residential location, age, marital status, and household size. Section two inquired about various meat consumption preferences and decision-making about household shopping, while the last enquired about their opinions on consumers’ perceptions of cultured meat. The perceptions were divided into implicit perception (consumers’ higher-order values pertaining to life, such as worldviews or cultures, personality traits and perceptions of novel products) (Wilks et al. 2019) and explicit perception (product-specific beliefs, views and thoughts) (Bryant and Barret 2020). The study investigated three worldviews. These described their view of (1) Science’s role in the food industry, (2) their level of responsibility in promoting responsible consumption, and (3) their tolerance for change or unfamiliar things. These worldviews were adopted from Wilks et al. (2019) and categorized as: (1) Attitude towards food Science and Technology (Trust vs. Distrust) (2) Level of communitarianism (Altruism vs egoism) (3) Tolerance (liberalism vs. illiberalism). According to Vidigal et al. (2015), worldviews facilitate the development of neophobic or neophilic personalities that enable the acceptance/rejection of new food products such as cultured meat. Similar to Wilks et al. (2019), the questions enquiring about consumers’ worldviews were extracted from Pliner and Hobden (1992). For estimating the explicit perceptions of cultured meat, opinion statements were organized into five categories that grouped (1) social and cultural, (2) economic, (3) quality, (4) health and safety, and (5) ethical concerns. These categories of concerns were constructed following a review of existing literature. Table 1 below shows the opinion statements asked in the survey that served as indicators of these categories of consumer psychology.
Responses to statements in Table 1 were elicited using a 5-point Likert Scale, calibrated as Strongly disagree = 1, Disagree = 2, Neither agree nor disagree = 3, Somewhat agree = 4 and Strongly agree = 5. Principal component analysis was then carried out on each group of opinion statements to draw a set of components or indices indicating the participants' dominant worldviews or perceptions of cultured meat. The Maximum Likelihood method in confirmatory factor analysis was utilised. This is a data dimension-reducing method that allows for the assessment of the fit between observed data and an a priori conceptualised, theoretically grounded model that specifies the hypothesised causal relations between latent factors and their observed indicator variables (Mueller and Hancock 2001). It does so by capturing the variance in the underlying data variables. Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) was used for data analysis. Varimax rotation was used to ensure that the derived components were uncorrelated. The average variance extracted (AVE) was used to confirm that the indicators of specific constructs share sufficient variance. During the extraction of the principal components (PCs), the Kaiser criterion (Kaiser 1970) of retaining factors with an eigenvalue of above one was utilised. Herewith, factor loadings above 0.5 were accepted and reported. The adequacy of the correlation between the opinion statements was measured using the Cronbach's alpha internal consistency test. The study aimed to acquire a score above 0.7, following Hair et al. (2013). The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) value was used to measure the sampling adequacy for the overall set of opinion statements. Following Hair et al. (2013), the acceptance threshold was set at 0.5. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was performed to test the variance in the analysed questions. A p value lower than 0.5 was considered acceptable.
The extracted PCs were regressed with other socio-economic factors as determinants of the willingness to try cultured meat burger patties. A hierarchical linear regression approach, first identifying the consumer psychology indicators and then the socio-economic predictors, was used. Given the order nature of the dependent variable, as shown in Table 2, an ordinal logit model could have been run, but there was no statistically significant difference between the categories created in the ordered variable. Hence, the model failed the goodness of fit test. As such, we explored clear delineations (No) vs. (unsure, yes-probably, and yes-definitely). Given that Neophobia focuses on the rejection of a product, we classified those who said they would definitely not or probably not willing to try vs. those who were unsure or said they would probably try. As such, the binary methodology lent itself well to hypothesis testing. The dependent variable was measured as a binary variable, so the binary logistic model was utilised. A binary variable also had the advantage of ensuring comparison with past studies’ findings. This model is expressed in Eq. 1.
where Yi is a binary variable coded as 1 = willing to try cultured meat and 0 = unsure or not willing to try cultured meatFootnote 3; Xi is a vector of the explanatory variables; β′ is a vector of parameters to be estimated, and F is a known function.
Table 2 describes the variables included in the regression model.
Results and discussion
Sample description
The average age of a survey respondent was 35.98, and the same sample was predominantly (55.69%) women (Table 3). The majority of the respondents were from Gauteng (44.31%), KwaZulu Natal (13.51%) and Western Cape (11.08%). The remaining six provinces accounted for less than 10% of the sample each. About sixty-three per cent (63.43%) of the respondents were of African descent, 19.88% were white, 4.25% were coloured, and 0.30% were Indian. One in every ten respondents was either vegetarian or vegan. As shown in Table 3, the survey overrepresented the female demographic group and Gauteng province while underrepresenting the black/African demographic group.
As shown in Table 4, about 40% of the respondents were well educated with a 3-year degree or better, 27.03% had received some form of vocational training, and the remaining 31.96% had attained a secondary school certificate or less. Most (26.61%) of the sample respondents fell under the middle-income bracket (R54 345–R863 906), where 20.82%, 29.48%, 16.10% and 6.99% were classified as low emerging, emerging, realised and upper middle class. Most of the respondents (54.71%) reported no intention to change their current meat consumption patterns, while 20.67% said they wanted to increase their consumption. About a quarter (24.62%) of the respondents had plans to reduce their meat consumption in the succeeding 12 months. Most respondents (58.44%) reported buying burger patties a few times (1–3) a month.
When asked about their knowledge of cultured meat, the majority of the study participants (42.6%) indicated that they had heard about cultured meat, 35.9% reported that they had heard about cultured meat but did not know what the term meant, and 21.5% of the respondents had never heard about cultured meat prior to this study. As shown in Fig. 1, 35.31% of the respondents said they were very willing; the majority said they would probably try it, 15.83% were unsure, while the remainder were either against the idea (5.02%) or strongly against (5.78%) the idea of trying it.
Implicit and explicit attitudes towards cultured meat burger patties
The results in Table 5 show that the survey respondents’ worldview was dominated by a strong distrust for Science and an unwillingness to promote responsible consumption in society actively. This is shown by the significant number of indicators in PC 1 showing negative perceptions of Science and Technology and egoism. This PC accounted for 28.58% of the perception data variation and had an eigenvalue of 2.286. This finding provides evidence of the scepticism towards Science reported by Wilks et al. (2019) and Lewisch and Riefler (2022). The results in PC1 also show that respondents would generally not choose to bear the costs of driving change required for sustainable lifestyles but believe that the government should coordinate efforts. The significant factor loadings in the opinion statements loaded on PC 2 also show a significant level of liberalism existed in the sample. This PC accounted for 14.02% of the variation in the sample’s perceptions and an eigenvalue of 1.121.
Table 6 indicates the relative ranking of concerns consumers had around cultured meat. Respondents were primarily concerned about the quality of cultured meat. This is indicated by the high number of meat quality opinion statements with statistically significant loadings on PC 1. As shown in PC 2, economic concerns were the second highest concern. This PC accounted for 8.927% of the variation in the data on consumer perceptions and had an eigenvalue of 1.785. The third-highest PC represented the largest variety of consumer perceptions. This PC, with an eigenvalue of 1.223, represented consumers’ ethical, health and safety concerns. The last PC represented social and cultural concerns. This PC had an eigenvalue of 1.104 and accounted for 5.679%.
Determinants of consumer acceptance of cultured meat burger patties
The results in Table 7 show how consumers’ product-specific implicit perceptions influenced consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat burger patties. This finding is consistent with prior expectations as other studies (Lewisch and Riefler 2022; Wilks et al. 2019; Piochi et al. 2022; Quevedo-Silva and Pereira 2022; Rombach et al. 2022; Lin-Hi et al. 2022; Van Loo et al. 2020; Hwang et al. 2020) also found that consumers often rely on preconceived attitudes from experiences with related products and high-order values to assist in making decisions on novel food products. However, Table 6 shows that the omnivore paradox is exhibited in the results as consumers’ worldviews had a mixed effect on their willingness to try cultured meat. On the one hand, consumers’ egoism and distrust in Science were found to deter the consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat (ß = -0.310; p value = 0.013). On the other hand, consumers’ liberalism (ß = − 0.030; p value = 0.244) positively influenced their willingness to try cultured meat. According to Rombach et al. (2022), the negative relationship with Science is born out of the need for self-preservation and is often driven by experience or evidence of negative externalities (negative side effects) of scientific products. This result is consistent with the findings reported by Zhang et al. (2020), Shaw and Iomaire (2019) and Wilks and Phillips (2017), which highlight distrust in food scientists and food safety authorities as potential barriers to consumer acceptance of cultured meat. Wilks et al. (2019) explain the positive relationship between liberalism and consumers’ propensity to accept cultured meat due to food curiosity. The results reported here support those that were reported by Rombach et al. (2022), Piochi et al. (2022), Arora et al. (2020), Grasso et al. (2019) and Bryant et al. (2019a, b), who found that having a liberal mindset, predicted cultured meat acceptance. As this is the case in this study, it can be recommended that marketing efforts be directed towards positioning cultured meat as a commonly consumed product instead of a Scientific innovation. This could reduce the concern of the possible failure that is associated with Scientific experiments. Bryant and Dillard (2019) suggest refraining from advertising cultured meat as a high-tech product that is accompanied by pictures of test tubes and Petri dishes as is portrayed in the media, but towards one that is more mundane. Interestingly, when comparing the size of the influence of the two worldviews in Table 6, the results indicate that consumers’ neophilic attitudes (liberalism) had a stronger influence than their neophobic attitudes (distrust in Science and egoism). Such a finding may predict a larger probability for consumers' acceptance of cultured meat as opposed to product than rejection tendency.
The results in Table 7 also indicate that the composite indices that measured consumers’ product-specific perceptions were significantly associated with the consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat. This means that cultured meat neophobia was a significant deterrent to trying cultured meat. The social and cultural concerns (ß = − 0.395; p value = 0.000) had the strongest influence. This was followed by the concern for possible economic disruptions (ß = − 0.305; p value = 0.008) and the health, food safety and ethical concerns (ß = − 0.395; p value = 0.000), which were second and third, respectively. Quality concerns (ß = − 0.223; p value = 0.045) were the lowest hindrance to trying cultured meat among the four explicit consumer perception variables. The order of influence of product-specific perceptions is different from those reported in past studies. De Oliveira Padilha et al. (2022) found that consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat in their research was affected by their perception of the meat quality was higher than the health-risk perception. The estimated level of influence is also markedly different as the former was reported to have a coefficient of 2.003 and the latter 1.709, while all coefficients in the current study have values below 0.5. None of the interaction terms between the implicit and explicit attitudes yielded any statistically significant results.
It is also interesting to note that matters found to be least concerning in the principal component analysis (social and cultural concerns) in Table 5 were found to have the strongest negative effect on consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat. This finding indicates how sensitive consumers can be when considering novel food products. This finding agrees with results reported by Bekker et al. (2017), who showed that consumers in African countries were more likely to reject cultured meat for social and cultural reasons. Koch et al. (2018) also explain that social and cultural concerns are far-reaching in some communities because violation of social norms can cause moral disgust and lead to consumers' subsequent rejection of food products. Meat quality concerns, the biggest concerns in the principal component analysis (Table 6), were among the lowest deterrents to consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat. These findings indicate that consumers are willing to overlook matters they consider most concerning and eat cultured meat. The high level of receptiveness by the consumers is also demonstrated by the fact that health and safety concerns, which often make up the most significant objections to consumer acceptance in past studies from elsewhere in the world, were the lowest in this study.
The results on consumer implicit and explicit perceptions highlight the opportunities that can be utilised and the pitfalls that can be avoided when trying to gather acceptance and momentum for cultured meat in the hamburger industry. For the implicit indicators, the fact that consumers had a domineering distrust of Science while also holding egoistic views indicates that the possible marketing that appeals to an altruistic nature in people may not be a very effective marketing approach in this market. Perhaps product value, which indicates a more direct benefit to the individual, would assist in providing more successful marketing penetration opportunities. Hamlin et al. (2022) also recommend using messages that are targeted at influencing consumers' immediate needs. This recommendation is similar to one given by Asioli et al. (2022), who advocated for marketing messages that indicate societal and personal benefits.
The results of the control variables were in line with prior expectations. As shown by the results in Table 6, survey participants who generally ate burgers were more willing to try cultured meat burgers than vegan/vegetarian counterparts, while individuals with less knowledge about cultured meat were less willing to try it. The results indicate that segmenting the South African market and developing targeted marketing efforts could be possible. This is because there were significant negative correlations between the willingness to try cultured meat and being male (ß = − 0.380; p value = 0.073), older (ß = − 0.019; p value = 0.027) and planning to reduce meat consumption (ß = − 0.393; p value = 0.016). Hence, the target markets for cultured meat hamburger patties could be: (1) females, (2) younger consumers and (3) individuals not planning to reduce meat consumption (meat lovers). These results agree with past studies that also found that females (Hwang et al. 2020; Shaw and Mac Con Iomaire 2019; Zhang et al. 2020), younger consumers (Lin-Hi et al. 2022; Piochi et al. 2022; Zhang et al. 2020), and those who a higher in attachment to meat (Bryant et al. 2019a, b; Arora et al. 2020; Wilks et al. 2019) were relatively more likely to try cultured meat. As expected, the preference for a non-meat-related name to be developed for faux meat was less willing to try cultured meat. This indicates that consumers who have a disdain for cultured meat are the ones who would have issues with the naming of the product.
Conclusion
This study investigated the antecedents of consumers’ willingness to try burger patties made from cultured meat in South Africa. It took a particular interest in psychological factors and examined implicit and explicit consumer perceptions. This advanced consumer research on cultured meat as it allowed a separation between the effect of consumers’ heuristics and product-specific attitudes while analysing their possible concurrent impact. The study’s results provided information that could assist in developing marketing campaigns that could lead to cultured meat burger patty adoption. Two key implications of the study’s findings are as follows: (1) messages that highlight cultured meat as a symbol of freedom of choice and inclusiveness could potentially encourage adoption, and (2) marketing messages indicating social acceptance are likely to encourage longer-term acceptance. Taken altogether, the results indicate that tactful marketing will be required to work around the neophobic attitudes and the product-specific perceptions present opportunities that could help consumers overcome their neophobia. As the findings gave a general indication of consumers’ openness to cultured meat burger patties, we recommend that policies be implemented to both facilitate the evolution of the protein supply industry and safeguard consumer welfare.
We want to acknowledge some limitations of this study. First, the sample used was not representative of the distribution of the demographic characteristics of South Africa. Although an adequate representation was achieved in the recruitment, high attrition, which occurred during data cleaning, resulted in the over-representation of more developed provinces. This made the sample more representative of the economically active population than the demographic spread of consumers. Second, the study’s results failed to find the influence of interaction between implicit and explicit perceptions. This can be investigated in future studies as this is consistent with theory and could provide insights into using this information in marketing. These terms' lack of significant influence indicates the consumers did not conflate their concerns about cultured meat with neophobic attitudes. Such a state of affairs may be present in other communities. Future studies can also investigate how the symbolism of freedom determined in this study can be used as a possible successful marketing theme for cultured meat. Lastly, the study used binary variables to measure willingness to try cultured meat. Although this was ensured in comparison with past studies’ findings, information was lost. We recommend that future studies use variables with even more categories in this variable’s measurement.
Availability of data and materials
Data are available on request.
Notes
Cultured meat is a newly developed food product consisting of muscle tissue grown from an animal’s stem cells within a laboratory environment. It is produced using animal stem cells using reverse engineering techniques (Post 2014).
- $${\text{Margin of Error}} = Z*\surd ((p*(1{-}p)/n$$
where Z′ is the Z*-value at a confidence level of 95%, p is the sample proportion, and n is the sample size.
The respondents were asked, “Are you willing to try cultured meat?”. The study participants responded No, definitely not = 1; No, probably not = 2; Unsure = 3; Yes, probably = 4 or Yes, definitely.
Abbreviations
- AVE:
-
Average variance extracted
- KMO:
-
Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin
- PC:
-
Principal components
References
Ang MYA, Ponte N, France C (2023) The influence of health star rating labels on plant-based foods: the moderating role of consumers’ believability. Food Qual Prefer. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2023.104827
Ajzen I (1991) The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 50(2):179–211
Arora RS, Brent DA, Jaenicke EC (2020) Is India ready for alt-meat? Preferences and willingness to pay for meat alternatives. Sustainability 12(11):4377. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12114377
Asioli D, Fuentes-Pila J, Alarcón S, Han J, Liu J, Hocquette J, Nayga RM (2022) Consumers’ valuation of cultured beef Burger: a multi-country investigation using choice experiments. Food Policy. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2022.102376
Bekker GA, Tobi H, Fischer ARH (2017) Meet meat: An explorative study on meat and cultured meat as seen by Chinese, Ethiopians and Dutch. Appetite 114:82–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.009
Bekker GA, Fischer ARH, Tobi H, van Trijp HCM (2021) Stuck in the middle with you: the role of similarity information on categorizing cultured meat. Food Qual Prefer. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104265
Bryant C, Dillard C (2019) The impact of framing on acceptance of cultured meat. Front Nutr. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2019.00103
Bryant C, Anderson JE, Asher KE, Green C, Gasteratos K (2019a) Strategies for overcoming aversion to unnaturalness: the case of clean meat. Meat Sci 154:37–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2019.04.004
Bryant C, Szejda K, Parekh N, Desphande V, Tse B (2019b) A survey of consumer perceptions of plant-based and clean meat in the USA, India, and China. Front Sustain 3:11
Bryant C, Barnet J (2020) Consumer acceptance of cultured meat: an updated review (2018–2020). Appl Sci 10:5201. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10155201
Cembalo L, Migliore G, Schifani G (2012) Consumers in postmodern society and alternative food networks. The organic food fairs case in Sicily. New Mediterian 11(3):41–49
Chasani A (2023) FDA clears lab-grown chicken as safe to eat. https://www.cbsnews.com/news/good-meat-lab-grown-chicken-safe-to-eat-fda/. Accessed 04 Apr 2023
De Koning W, Dean D, Vriesekoop F, Aguiar LK, Anderson M, Mongondry P, Boereboom A (2020) Drivers and inhibitors in the acceptance of meat alternatives: the case of plant and insect-based proteins. Foods 9(9):1–18. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9091292
De Oliveira Padilha LG, MalekL UWJ (2022) Consumers’ attitudes towards lab-grown meat, conventionally raised meat and plant-based protein alternatives. Food Qual Prefer. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104573
Fischler C (1988) Food, self and identity. Soc Sci Inf 27(2):275–292
Flaws B (2019) Fonterra is investing in artificial meat, but would you eat it?. Retrieved from https://www.stuff.co.nz/business/111546089/51tago51d51-is-investing-in-labgrown-meat-but-would-you-eat-it
Fountain H (2013) Building a $325,000 Burger. May 12. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2013/05/14/science/engineering-the-325000-in-vitro-burger.html
Future Meat (2021) Future Meat website. In. de Oliveira Padilha LG, Malek L, Umberger WJ. 2021. Food choice drivers of potential lab-grown meat consumers in Australia. British Food Journal, 123(9), 3014–3031
Gawronski B, Bodenhausen GV (2006) Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol Bull 132(5):692–731. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692
Gomez-Luciano CA, de Aguiar LK, Vriesekoop K, Urbano B (2019) Consumers’ willingness to purchase three alternatives to meat proteins in the United Kingdom, Spain, Brazil and the Dominican Republic. Food Qual and Pref. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103732
Grasso A, Hung Y, Olthof MR, Verbeke W, Brouwer IA (2019) Older consumers’ readiness to accept alternative, more sustainable protein sources in the European Union. Nutrients 11:1904
Hair JF, Black CW, Babin BJ, Anderson RE (2013) Multivariate data analysis, 7th edn. Pearson, London
Hamlin R, McNeil LS, Sim J (2022) Food neophobia, food choice and the details of cultured meat. Meat Sci. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2022.108964
Holland K (2023) Italy proposes to ban production of cultivated meat and ‘synthetic foods’. https://foodmatterslive.com/article/italy-proposes-ban-production-cultivated-meat-synthetic-foods/. Accessed 04 Apr 2023
Hwang J, You J, Moon J, Jeong J (2020) Factors affecting consumers’ alternative meats buying intentions: plant-based meat alternative and cultured meat. Sustainability 12(14):5662. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12145662
Kaiser HF (1970) A second-generation Little Jiffy. Psychometrika 35:401–415
Koch J, van Ittersum K, Bolderdijk W (2018) Disgusting? No, just different. Understanding consumer skepticism towards sustainable food innovations. Assoc Consum Res 46:1–12
Lewisch L, Riefler P (2022) Behavioural intention towards cultured meat: the role of personal values, domain-specific innovativeness and distrust for Science. Br Food J. https://doi.org/10.1108/BFJ-03-2022-0270
Li H, Van Loo EJ, van Trijp HCM, Chen J, Bai J (2023) Will cultured meat be served on Chinese tables? A study of consumer attitudes and intensions about cultured meat in China. Meat Sci. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2022.109081
Lin-Hi N, Schäfer K, Blumberg I, Hollands L (2022) The omnivore’s paradox and consumer acceptance of cultured meat: an experimental investigation into the role of perceived organizational competence and excitement. J Clean Prod 338:130593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2022.130593
Mueller RO, Hancock GR (2001) Factor analysis and latent structure, confirmatory. Int Encycl Soc Behav Sci. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/00426-5
Piochi M, Micheloni M, Torri L (2022) Effect of informative claims on the attitude of Italian consumers towards cultured meat and relationship among variables used in an explicit approach. Food Res Int 151:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2021.110881
Pliner P, Hobden K (1992) Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 19:105–120
Post MJ (2014) Cultured beef: medical technology to produce food. J Sci Food Agric 94(6):1039–1041
Quevedo-Silva F, Pereira JB (2022) Factors affecting consumers’ cultivated meat purchase intentions. Sustainability 14:12501. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141912501
Rombach M, Dean D, Vriesekoop F, de Koning W, Aguiar LK, Anderson M, Mongondry P, Oppong-Gyamfi M, Urbano B, Gómez Luciano CA, Hao W, Eastwick E, Jiang ZV, Boereboom A (2022) Is cultured meat a promising consumer alternative? Exploring key factors determining consumer’s willingness to try, buy and pay a premium for cultured meat. Appetite. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2022.106307
Siegrist M, Hartmann C (2020) Perceived naturalness, disgust, trust and food neophobia as predictors of cultured meat acceptance in ten countries. Appetite. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104814
Shaw EM, Mac Con Iomaire I (2019) A comparative analysis of the attitudes of rural and urban consumers towards cultured meat. Br Food J 121(8):1782–1800
Stark V (2022) South African company first to produce meat in lab. Science and Health. https://www.voanews.com/a/south-african-company-first-in-africa-to-produce-meat-in-lab/6618870.html. Accessed 4 Nov 2022
Starowicz M, Poznar KK, Zieliński H (2020) What are the main sensory attributes that determine the acceptance of meat alternatives? Curr Opin Food Sci. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cofs.2022.100924
StatsSA (2022) National census. Department of Statistics South Africa. https://www.statssa.gov.za/?p=15473. Accessed 15 Sept 2023
Stowe K, Venter I (2020) Factors influencing a healthier fast food choice intention after the provision of energy and extended nutritional information among working young adults in the city of Cape Town, South Africa. J Consum Sci 5(1):19–39
Szejda K, Stumpe M, Raal L, Tapscott CE (2021) South African consumer adoption of plant-based and cultivated meat: a segmentation study. Front Sustain Food Syst 5:744199. https://doi.org/10.3389/fsufs.2021.744199
Terazono E (2021) May 6. Lab-grown chicken start-up slashes production costs. Financial Times. https://www.ft.com/content/ae4dd452-f3e0-4a38-a29d-3516c5280bc7
The Guardian (2020) No-kill, lab-grown meat to go on sale for first time. https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2020/dec/02/no-kill-lab-grown-meat-to-go-on-sale-for-first-time
Van Loo EJ, Caputo V, Lusk JL (2020) Consumer preferences for farm-raised meat, lab-grown meat, and plant-based meat alternatives: does information or brand matter? Food Policy. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2020.101931
Verneau F, Caracciolo F, Coppola A, Lombadi P (2014) Consumer fears and familiarity of processed food. The value of information provided by the FTNS. Appetite 73:140–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.11.004
Vidigal MCTR, Minim VPR, Simiqueli AA, Souza PHP, Balbino DF, Minim LA (2015) Food technology neophobia and consumer attitudes toward foods produced by new and conventional technologies: a case study in Brazil. LWT Food Sci Technol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lwt.2014.10.058
Wilks M, Phillips CJC (2017) Attitudes to in vitro meat: a survey of potential consumers in the United States. PLoS ONE 12(2):1–14. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171904
Wilks M, Phillips CJC, Fielding K, Hornsey MJ (2019) Testing potential psychological predictors of attitudes towards cultured meat. Appetite 136:137–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.01.027
Zhang M, Li L, Bai J (2020) Consumer acceptance of cultured meat in urban areas of three cities in China. Food Control 118:107390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodcont.2020.107390
Acknowledgements
None.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
CT was involved in the study’s conceptualization, data collection, data analysis and writing of the manuscript. LN was involved in the study’s conceptualization, data collection and writing of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Tsvakirai, C.Z., Nalley, L.L. The coexistence of psychological drivers and deterrents of consumers’ willingness to try cultured meat hamburger patties: evidence from South Africa. Agric Econ 11, 52 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40100-023-00293-4
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40100-023-00293-4